Learning Objectives
- Define the process of sensation and perception.
- Explore Gestalt principles in perception.
- Differentiate between types of bottom-up processes.
- Compare various types of top-down processes.
- Discuss the use of composite faces in understanding face perception.
- Summarize the concept of direct perception.
- Analyze the differences among types of visual agnosias.
Sensation is the process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energies from our environment. This initial stage involves detecting physical stimuli like light, sound, and touch, and converting these stimuli into neural signals.
Perception follows sensation and is the process of organizing and interpreting sensory information to give it meaning. The classic approach to perception starts with objects in the real world, known as distal stimuli. These stimuli are detected by our sensory organs and create proximal stimuli, which are the sensory inputs registered by our senses. The brain then interprets these proximal stimuli to form a percept, which is the meaningful interpretation of the sensory information.
Pattern Recognition is a related process where the brain identifies and categorizes objects and patterns within the perceptual data, helping us to recognize familiar shapes, sounds, and other sensory inputs.
The Law of Pragnanz and Gestalt Principles
The Law of Pragnanz, also known as the law of simplicity, is a fundamental principle in Gestalt psychology. It states that our minds tend to perceive ambiguous or complex images in the simplest form possible. This means that we organize our perceptions into the most stable and simplest shapes or forms. This overarching principle subsumes several specific Gestalt principles, each explaining different aspects of how we achieve simplicity and stability in our perceptions:
- Figure-Ground Relationship:
- We instinctively separate objects (figures) from their background (ground).
- This helps us to focus on and identify objects within a scene.
The depicted version of Rubin’s vase can be seen as the black profiles of two people looking towards each other or as a white vase, but not both.
- Proximity:
- Elements that are close to each other are perceived as a group.
- This principle helps simplify the scene by reducing the number of separate elements we need to process.
For example, in the figure illustrating the law of proximity, there are 72 circles, but we perceive the collection of circles in groups. Specifically, we perceive that there is a group of 36 circles on the left side of the image and three groups of 12 circles on the right side of the image. This law is often used in advertising logos to emphasize which aspects of events are associated.
- Similarity:
- Items that are similar in appearance are grouped together.
- This allows us to perceive complex environments as more organized and less chaotic.
For example, the figure illustrating the law of similarity portrays 36 circles all equal distance apart from one another forming a square. In this depiction, 18 of the circles are shaded dark, and 18 of the circles are shaded light. We perceive the dark circles as grouped together and the light circles as grouped together, forming six horizontal lines within the square of circles. This perception of lines is due to the law of similarity
- Continuity:
- We prefer continuous lines and patterns rather than abrupt changes or discontinuities.
- This helps us to see smooth paths and connected shapes.
For example, the figure depicting the law of continuity shows a configuration of two crossed keys. When the image is perceived, we tend to perceive the key in the background as a single uninterrupted key instead of two separate halves of a ke
- Closure:
- Our brains tend to fill in gaps to create a complete, whole object.
- This means we often perceive incomplete shapes as complete ones.
For example, the figure that depicts the law of closure portrays what we perceive as a circle on the left side of the image and a rectangle on the right side of the image. However, gaps are present in the shapes. If the law of closure did not exist, the image would depict an assortment of different lines with different lengths, rotations, and curvatures—but with the law of closure, we perceptually combine the lines into whole shapes.
- Symmetry and Order:
For example, the figure depicting the law of symmetry shows a configuration of square and curled brackets. When the image is perceived, we tend to observe three pairs of symmetrical brackets rather than six individual brackets.
- We prefer to see symmetrical shapes and organized structures.
- Symmetry and orderliness contribute to the perception of stability and simplicity.
Application of the Law of Pragnanz
The Law of Pragnanz explains why we naturally organize our perceptions in a way that reduces cognitive load and increases efficiency. For instance, when looking at a complex image, our brain quickly identifies patterns, groups similar objects, and fills in missing parts to form a coherent and easily understandable scene. This process is crucial for effective navigation and interaction with our environment.
Understanding these principles and the Law of Pragnanz provides insight into human perception and is applied in various fields, including design, art, and user interface development, to create visuals that are intuitive and easy to comprehend.
Bottom-up and top-down processes
Bottom-up and top-down processes work together to create a cohesive perception of the world, integrating raw sensory input with our cognitive frameworks and experiences.
Types of Bottom-Up Processes
Bottom-up processes are data-driven and rely on incoming sensory information to build up a perception. Here are the primary types:
- Feature Detection:
- Involves the detection of basic visual features like edges, colors, and shapes.
- Specific neurons in the visual cortex respond to these basic features, which are then combined to form a more complex perception.
- Template Matching:
- Compares sensory input to stored templates or patterns in memory.
- When a match is found, the object is recognized.
- Prototype Matching:
- Involves matching the sensory input to an idealized, average representation of an object (a prototype).
- This allows for recognition even when the actual object is not an exact match to the stored prototype.
- Recognition-by-Components:
- Proposes that objects are recognized by identifying basic shapes (geons) and their arrangements.
- Geons are simple 3D shapes like cylinders, cones, and blocks, which combine to form complex objects.
Types of Top-Down Processes
Top-down processes are concept-driven and rely on prior knowledge, expectations, and experiences to interpret sensory information. Here are the main types:
- Contextual Effects:
- The context in which a stimulus is encountered influences how it is perceived.
- For example, a letter in a word is easier to identify than a letter presented in isolation.
- Expectancy and Hypotheses:
- Our expectations and hypotheses about what we are likely to see can influence perception.
- This is often based on past experiences or knowledge about a situation.
- Schemas and Scripts:
- Schemas are organized knowledge structures about concepts and objects.
- Scripts are knowledge structures about sequences of events or actions.
- Both help in predicting and interpreting sensory information based on what typically happens in familiar situations.
- Perceptual Set:
- A mental predisposition to perceive one thing and not another.
- Influenced by factors such as culture, emotions, and motivations.
- Attention:
- Focusing cognitive resources on specific aspects of the sensory input while ignoring others.
- Attention can be directed by top-down processes, such as when searching for a friend in a crowd.
Word Superiority Effect: A Classic Example of Top-Down Processing
The word superiority effect is a phenomenon in cognitive psychology that illustrates how top-down processing influences our perception and recognition of visual stimuli, particularly letters and words. Here’s a detailed explanation of this effect and its implications:
Definition of Word Superiority Effect
- Definition: The word superiority effect refers to the finding that people are better and faster at recognizing letters presented within words compared to letters presented in isolation or within non-word contexts.
- Key Components:
- Contextual Priming: Words provide a context that facilitates the recognition of individual letters within them.
- Top-Down Influence: Prior knowledge about word structures and expectations guide the perception of letters, enhancing processing efficiency.
Mechanism of the Word Superiority Effect
- Perceptual Enhancement: When a word is presented, the brain uses top-down processing to anticipate and predict the letters that are likely to follow based on context and language rules.
- Feedback Mechanism: The recognition of the whole word provides feedback that helps in identifying and verifying the individual letters more quickly and accurately.
Experimental Evidence
- Experimental Setup: In experiments, participants are typically shown letters either in isolation, within non-words, or within real words. They are asked to identify or discriminate these letters under different conditions.
- Findings: Researchers consistently find that reaction times and accuracy are highest when letters are presented within real words compared to non-words or isolated letters. This demonstrates the facilitative effect of context on letter perception.
Implications of the Word Superiority Effect
- Reading Ability: The word superiority effect highlights how our ability to read fluently relies on top-down processing mechanisms. Our knowledge of word structures and language rules helps us recognize and comprehend text quickly.
- Cognitive Processing: Understanding this effect contributes to our knowledge of how cognitive processes interact during perception. It underscores the importance of context and expectations in shaping how we interpret sensory input.
Practical Applications
- Education: Educators can leverage the word superiority effect to design effective reading instruction programs that emphasize whole-word recognition alongside phonetic decoding.
- User Interface Design: Designers can use principles from the word superiority effect to create user interfaces that enhance readability and ease of information processing.
In summary, the word superiority effect exemplifies top-down processing in perception by demonstrating how our knowledge of language and word structures influences how we perceive and recognize visual stimuli such as letters and words. This phenomenon provides valuable insights into the complex interplay between sensory information and cognitive expectations in human perception.
Featural Analysis Models of Perception
Featural Analysis Models are based on the idea that we recognize objects by breaking them down into their constituent features. These models assume that perception involves analyzing a stimulus into its parts, or features, to recognize the whole object. Here’s a detailed look at how these models function and their significance in the perception process:
- Detection of Basic Features:
- Involves identifying simple, elemental features of a stimulus such as lines, edges, colors, orientations, and shapes.
- Feature detectors, specialized neurons in the visual cortex, respond selectively to specific types of stimuli.
- Combination of Features:
- After detecting basic features, the brain combines them to form a coherent representation of the whole object.
- This integration process is essential for recognizing complex objects from their simpler components.
- Feature Integration Theory:
- Proposed by Anne Treisman, this theory suggests that object perception occurs in two stages:
- Pre-attentive Stage: Features are detected automatically and in parallel without conscious effort.
- Focused Attention Stage: Features are combined using attention to form a whole object, allowing for object recognition.
- Recognition-by-Components (RBC) Theory:
- Proposed by Irving Biederman, RBC theory posits that objects are recognized by identifying their basic geometric shapes (geons) and their spatial relationships.
- Geons are simple 3D shapes like cylinders, cones, and blocks that can be combined in various ways to form complex objects.
Importance of Featural Analysis
Featural analysis models provide a framework for understanding how the brain processes complex visual information. By breaking down stimuli into manageable parts, these models explain how we can recognize a vast array of objects quickly and efficiently. This approach also helps in understanding various visual phenomena and has applications in fields such as computer vision, artificial intelligence, and cognitive psychology.
Examples of Featural Analysis in Practice
- Reading:
- Recognizing letters involves detecting their distinct features, such as lines and curves, and then combining them to identify the letter and eventually the word.
- Face Recognition:
- Involves detecting features like eyes, nose, and mouth, and their spatial arrangement to recognize a face.
- Object Recognition:
- Identifying a chair involves recognizing features like legs, seat, and backrest, and integrating these features to perceive the whole chair.
Featural analysis models highlight the importance of both feature detection and integration in the perceptual process, providing a comprehensive understanding of how we make sense of the complex visual world.
Posner and Keele’s Research on Prototype Learning
Research conducted by Posner and Keele has contributed significantly to our understanding of how people learn and categorize new stimuli, particularly through the concept of prototypes. Here’s an overview of their findings and the implications of prototype learning:
- Prototype Theory:
- Prototype theory suggests that people form mental representations, or prototypes, that capture the typical features of a category.
- These prototypes are abstract representations derived from common features of category members.
- Ease of Learning:
- Posner and Keele found that people learn prototypes of new classes of stimuli relatively easily.
- When presented with new stimuli that belong to a certain category, individuals quickly recognize and categorize them based on similarities to the prototype.
- Prototype Categorization:
- Prototypes are considered typical examples of a category and are easier to categorize than atypical or less representative examples.
- For instance, when learning about birds, a prototype might include features like wings, beak, and feathers, making it easier to recognize a new bird that shares these features.
- Exemplar Variability:
- While prototypes represent the average features of a category, individual exemplars (specific instances) may vary in their similarity to the prototype.
- People can categorize both prototypical and atypical exemplars, but prototypical examples are categorized more quickly and confidently.
Implications of Prototype Learning
- Cognitive Efficiency: Learning prototypes allows for cognitive efficiency because it simplifies the categorization process. Instead of memorizing every instance, people generalize based on shared features.
- Generalization: Prototypes facilitate the generalization of knowledge to new instances. Once a prototype is established, similar stimuli are recognized and categorized more efficiently.
- Application in Education and Training: Understanding prototype learning can inform educational practices by emphasizing the presentation of clear prototypes in teaching new concepts. Similarly, in training settings, presenting clear examples of desired behaviors can enhance learning and performance.
Practical Applications
- Design and Marketing: Designers and marketers use prototype theory to create products and advertisements that embody the typical features and characteristics of a desired category, making them more recognizable and appealing to consumers.
- Psychological Assessment: Prototypes are used in psychological assessment to understand how people categorize and perceive stimuli, providing insights into cognitive processes and individual differences.
In summary, Posner and Keele’s research underscores the importance of prototype learning in cognitive processes, demonstrating that prototypes facilitate learning, categorization, and generalization of new stimuli by capturing the essence of a category through its most representative features.
David Marr’s model of perception is a seminal framework in cognitive science that integrates both bottom-up and top-down processes to understand how the brain processes visual information. Marr, a neuroscientist and cognitive psychologist, proposed a computational theory of vision in his influential book “Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information” (1982). Here’s how Marr’s model incorporates these processes:
Marr’s Levels of Analysis
Marr proposed a three-level hierarchy to explain visual perception, each level corresponding to a different aspect of information processing:
- Computational Level:
- This level defines the problem that the visual system must solve and the goal of perception.
- It focuses on what information needs to be extracted from the visual scene to achieve perception.
- Algorithmic Level:
- At this level, Marr describes the specific algorithms or processes used to solve the computational problems.
- It outlines how raw sensory data are processed to extract meaningful features and structures from the visual input.
- Implementation Level:
- This level concerns the physical implementation of the algorithms in the brain’s neural hardware.
- It addresses how neural mechanisms and circuits carry out the computations described at the algorithmic level.
Integration of Bottom-Up and Top-Down Processes
- Bottom-Up Processes: Marr’s model incorporates bottom-up processes by emphasizing the importance of early visual processing mechanisms that analyze raw sensory inputs (such as edges, colors, and shapes). These low-level processes involve feature detection and integration to form initial representations of the visual scene.
- Top-Down Processes: Marr also integrates top-down processes by recognizing the role of higher-level cognitive factors, such as expectations, knowledge, and context, in shaping perception. These processes influence how incoming sensory information is interpreted and categorized based on prior experience and cognitive biases.
Contributions to Understanding Perception
Marr’s model has been influential in cognitive science and computer vision for several reasons:
- Computational Clarity: It provides a clear and structured framework for understanding how complex cognitive functions like perception can be decomposed into computationally tractable problems.
- Bridge between Neuroscience and Psychology: Marr’s approach bridges the gap between neural mechanisms studied in neuroscience and behavioral phenomena observed in psychology by linking algorithmic processes to their neural implementation.
- Inspiration for AI and Machine Vision: Marr’s ideas have inspired research in artificial intelligence, particularly in developing algorithms for computer vision that mimic human perceptual processes.
In conclusion, David Marr’s model of perception is notable for its comprehensive integration of both bottom-up (sensory-driven) and top-down (cognition-driven) processes, providing a foundational framework for understanding how the brain perceives and interprets visual information.
Perceptual Learning and Gibson-Gibson Study
Perceptual learning refers to the improvement in perceptual performance with practice and experience. It involves changes in perception that result from repeated exposure to stimuli, leading to enhanced sensitivity and discrimination of features. Here’s a closer look at perceptual learning and the study by Gibson and Gibson:
Perceptual Learning
- Definition: Perceptual learning refers to the process through which our ability to perceive sensory information improves due to practice and experience. It can occur across various sensory modalities, including vision, hearing, touch, and more.
- Mechanisms: Perceptual learning involves changes in the brain’s neural processes, such as increased sensitivity of neurons involved in detecting specific features or patterns, improved connectivity between brain regions, and more efficient allocation of attentional resources.
- Applications: Perceptual learning has practical applications in education, rehabilitation, and skill training. For example, it can enhance reading skills, improve performance in visual tasks, and aid in recovering sensory functions after injury.
Gibson and Gibson Study
- Study Context: Eleanor and James J. Gibson conducted research to investigate how perceptual learning occurs in children and adults using a card-identification task.
- Methodology: Participants, including children and adults, were tasked with identifying and differentiating cards with varying features over multiple sessions.
- Findings: They observed that both children and adults showed improvement in performance over time. Specifically, participants learned to notice more features of the stimuli as they became more familiar with the task.
- Implications: The study demonstrated that perceptual learning involves the ability to detect and discriminate finer details or features of stimuli with practice. This improved sensitivity contributes to enhanced perceptual abilities and more accurate perception over time.
In summary, perceptual learning, as illustrated by the Gibson and Gibson study, highlights the dynamic nature of perception and its ability to adapt and improve with experience. This phenomenon underscores the plasticity of the human brain and its capacity to refine sensory processing mechanisms through repeated exposure and practice. Understanding perceptual learning is crucial for optimizing learning strategies, enhancing perceptual skills, and developing effective interventions in various domains of cognition and behavior.
Composite Faces and Face Perception
The phenomenon of composite faces is used in cognitive psychology to investigate and understand the process of face perception, particularly how we recognize and integrate facial features into a coherent whole. Here’s how composite faces contribute to our understanding of face perception:
- Composite Face Effect:
- In experiments involving composite faces, researchers create images where the top and bottom halves of different faces are combined.
- Participants are asked to judge the identity or expression of these composite faces.
- Findings:
- Participants often perceive the composite face as having features that are a blend of the top and bottom halves, even though they are from different faces.
- This phenomenon demonstrates that when processing faces, we integrate facial features holistically rather than independently.
- Holistic Processing:
- Holistic processing refers to the tendency to perceive and process faces as integrated wholes rather than collections of independent parts.
- The composite face effect suggests that facial features are perceived in relation to one another, influencing how we identify individuals or interpret facial expressions.
- Implications:
- Understanding the composite face effect helps researchers elucidate how the brain processes facial information.
- It informs theories about face perception, such as the idea that faces are processed in a specialized manner distinct from other visual stimuli.
- Applications:
- This research has practical applications in fields such as forensic science, eyewitness testimony, and facial recognition technology.
- It underscores the importance of understanding how facial features are encoded and recognized in different contexts.
Direct Perception
Direct perception is a theoretical perspective in perceptual psychology that contrasts with the traditional view of perception as a process of constructing mental representations based on sensory input. Here’s a summary of direct perception:
- Definition:
- Direct perception proposes that perception occurs immediately and effortlessly through direct coupling between the sensory information available in the environment and our perceptual systems.
- According to this view, perception is not mediated by internal representations or cognitive processes but is instead a direct and inseparable interaction between the perceiver and the environment.
- Gibsonian Perspective:
- Direct perception is closely associated with the ecological approach to perception proposed by J.J. Gibson.
- Gibson argued that perception is attuned to the affordances of the environment, meaning that perceivers directly perceive actionable information (such as opportunities for action) in their surroundings.
- Key Concepts:
- Affordances: The properties of objects and environments that allow for specific actions or interactions.
- Information Pickup: The idea that perceivers pick up perceptual information directly from the environment without needing to construct mental representations.
- Examples:
- When you reach for a cup, direct perception suggests that your ability to grasp it is based on perceiving its size, shape, and orientation in relation to your hand, rather than on mentally reconstructing its visual image.
- Criticism and Debate:
- Critics argue that while direct perception emphasizes the importance of immediate sensory information, cognitive processes and internal representations still play significant roles in perception, particularly in complex perceptual tasks.
In summary, direct perception offers a perspective that challenges traditional views of perception by emphasizing the immediacy and directness of perceptual experience. It highlights the dynamic interaction between organisms and their environment, suggesting that perception involves more than just passive reception of sensory stimuli but an active engagement with the affordances and opportunities presented by the world around us.
Disruptions of Perception: Visual Agnosias
Visual agnosia refers to a condition where an individual has difficulty recognizing or interpreting visual stimuli despite intact visual perception. There are different types of visual agnosias, each affecting specific aspects of visual recognition. Here’s a comparison and contrast of the main types:
1. Apperceptive Agnosia
- Definition: Apperceptive agnosia is characterized by a failure to perceive and recognize objects due to deficits in basic perceptual processes.
- Features:
- Impaired Object Recognition: Individuals with apperceptive agnosia cannot correctly perceive the shape, size, or form of objects, even though their visual acuity and basic visual functions (like color and motion perception) may be intact.
- Copy and Matching Tasks: They struggle with tasks that involve copying drawings or matching objects based on their visual features.
- Causes: Often caused by damage to the occipital and parietal lobes or their connections.
2. Associative Agnosia
- Definition: Associative agnosia involves a deficit in recognizing objects despite intact perception of their basic features.
- Features:
- Intact Perception, Impaired Recognition: Individuals can perceive the details of objects but cannot assign meaning to them or identify them.
- Semantic Memory Deficit: Difficulty accessing stored knowledge about objects and their functions.
- Causes: Typically results from damage to the temporal lobes or connections between the occipital, temporal, and parietal lobes.
3. Integrative Agnosia
- Definition: Integrative agnosia is characterized by difficulty perceiving the relationship between parts of an object or understanding how parts combine to form a whole.
- Features:
- Impaired Gestalt Perception: Difficulty perceiving the overall shape or structure of objects.
- Segmentation Errors: Objects may be seen as fragmented or disorganized, making it hard to recognize them as coherent entities.
- Causes: Often associated with lesions affecting the parietal-occipital junction or connections within the visual system.
Prosopagnosia is a type of visual agnosia specifically characterized by difficulty in recognizing faces, including familiar faces of family members, friends, or celebrities, despite intact vision and general cognitive functions. Here’s an exploration of prosopagnosia, its features, associated brain regions, and implications:
Features of Prosopagnosia
- Impaired Face Recognition: Individuals with prosopagnosia have difficulty identifying and recognizing faces.
- Faces vs. Objects: Recognition deficits are specific to faces; these individuals can typically recognize objects, places, and other non-face stimuli without significant impairment.
- Difficulty with Facial Features: They may struggle to discern facial features such as eyes, nose, mouth, and overall facial structure.
- Visual Unfamiliarity: Faces may appear unfamiliar or indistinguishable, even when seen repeatedly.
Types of Prosopagnosia
- Developmental Prosopagnosia: Occurs from early childhood without any apparent brain injury or lesion.
- Acquired Prosopagnosia: Results from brain injury, typically affecting specific brain regions involved in face processing.
Associated Brain Region
- Right Hemisphere Damage: Prosopagnosia is often associated with lesions or damage to the right hemisphere of the brain, particularly in areas such as:
- Fusiform Face Area (FFA): Located in the fusiform gyrus of the temporal lobe, the FFA is specialized for face perception and processing.
- Occipital Face Area (OFA): Another region involved in early stages of face processing, located in the occipital lobe.
- Temporal Cortex: Various parts of the temporal cortex, including regions important for integrating facial features and associating them with identity.
Implications and Challenges
- Social and Emotional Impact: Prosopagnosia can lead to social awkwardness, difficulty in maintaining relationships, and challenges in daily life activities that rely heavily on recognizing faces.
- Compensatory Strategies: Individuals with prosopagnosia may develop compensatory strategies, such as recognizing people by their voice, gait, or contextual clues.
Diagnosis and Management
- Diagnostic Tests: Diagnosis often involves specialized tests of face recognition abilities and may include neuroimaging to identify underlying brain damage.
- Management: Currently, there is no cure for prosopagnosia, but strategies such as facial feature analysis training and cognitive behavioral therapy can help individuals cope with the condition.
In conclusion, prosopagnosia is a specific form of visual agnosia characterized by impaired face recognition, often associated with damage to the right hemisphere of the brain, particularly in regions specialized for processing faces. Understanding the neural basis and features of prosopagnosia is crucial for developing effective interventions and support strategies for affected individuals.
Comparison and Contrast
- Common Features: All types of visual agnosia involve impairment in recognizing visual stimuli despite intact perceptual abilities.
- Distinguishing Features:
- Perceptual vs. Recognition Deficits: Apperceptive agnosia involves perceptual deficits (in seeing the object itself), while associative agnosia involves recognition deficits (in understanding what the object is).
- Semantic Memory Involvement: Associative agnosia specifically affects access to semantic memory, which is less of a factor in apperceptive and integrative agnosias.
- Gestalt vs. Part-Based Deficits: Integrative agnosia involves difficulties in integrating parts into a whole (gestalt perception), whereas apperceptive and associative agnosias may focus more on perceiving individual parts or recognizing their meaning.
- Neurological Basis: Each type of agnosia is associated with damage to specific brain regions or their connections, which determines the nature of the perceptual or recognition deficit.
In summary, while all types of visual agnosia involve deficits in visual recognition, they differ in terms of the specific perceptual or recognition processes that are impaired and the underlying brain areas affected. Understanding these distinctions helps in diagnosing and addressing the challenges faced by individuals with visual agnosia.
Key Takeaways
- Sensation: The process of detecting physical stimuli from the environment and transmitting this information to the brain.
- Perception: The process of interpreting and organizing sensory information to give it meaning.
- Gestalt Principles: Principles of perceptual organization that describe how humans perceive and group visual stimuli into meaningful patterns.
- Bottom-Up Processing: Processing sensory information starting from basic elements and building up to a complete perception.
- Top-Down Processing: Processing that starts with higher-level cognitive processes, such as expectations and prior knowledge, which influence perception.
- Composite Faces: Experimental stimuli created by combining parts of different faces to study how we perceive and recognize faces.
- Direct Perception: A theoretical perspective suggesting that perception occurs directly through the environment’s information without requiring internal representations.
- Visual Agnosias: Disorders characterized by difficulty recognizing and interpreting visual stimuli despite intact sensory abilities.
- Apperceptive Agnosia: A type of visual agnosia involving perceptual deficits in recognizing objects and shapes.
- Associative Agnosia: A type of visual agnosia involving deficits in recognizing and assigning meaning to objects.
- Integrative Agnosia: A type of visual agnosia involving difficulty perceiving the relationship between parts of an object.
- Prosopagnosia: A specific type of visual agnosia characterized by an inability to recognize familiar faces.
Candela Citations
Public domain content
Lumen Learning authored content
- I used ChatGPT with all learning Objectives. Authored by: Sonja Miller. Project: Creation of OER for Cognitive Psychology Class. License: CC BY: Attribution