how can we use it to understand and respond to our patients’ needs?
As Hill et al. point out in their powerful paper [1], there is often a difference between objective clinical and radiographic evidence of musculoskeletal disease activity or severity and the experience of pain, other symptoms and functional ability reported by the patient. For the patient, the greatest impact of the disease lies in the effect it has on their ability to continue with a ‘normal’ daily life and this will necessarily be their focus of interest. It is, however, increasingly important for us as researchers and healthcare professionals to understand how the perceptions, experience and impact of having a musculoskeletal condition might influence a patient’s interpretation and response to it, so that we, in turn, can respond more appropriately.
The model used by Hill et al. in their paper [1] to investigate these issues is varyingly known as the Illness Perceptions Model, the Illness Representations Model, the Self-Regulatory Model, the Parallel Process Model, the Common-Sense Model of self-regulation or simply as Leventhal’s model, after Howard Leventhal and colleagues [2–7] who championed research in this field. For ease, we will refer to it as the Common-Sense Model (CSM).
Leventhal started his research in the late 1960s and early 1970s looking at how fear messages in relatively acute situations might lead to people taking health-promoting actions such as wearing seat belts or giving up smoking [3]. He and his colleagues found that different types of information were needed to influence both attitudes and actions to a perceived threat to health and well-being, and these only lasted for short periods of time. In extending their initial model, Leventhal and colleagues wanted to know what adaptations and coping efforts might need to be made and maintained in those experiencing chronic illness. They proposed a hierarchically organized model of an adaptive system featuring the three main constructs of (i) ‘representation’ of the illness experience that might guide (ii) action planning or ‘coping’ responses and performance of these, followed by (iii) ‘appraisal,’ or monitoring of the success or failure of coping efforts [7]. The model has many similarities with other theories of problem-solving behavior, such as the transactional model of stress and coping [8], wherein illness can be conceptualized as a stressful experience. A novel feature of Leventhal’s proposition was to delineate the active parallel cognitive processing of how people regulate their responses both to ‘illness danger’ (‘What is this health threat, what can I objectively do about it?’) and to the person’s regulation of ‘emotional control’ (‘How do I feel about it, what can I do to make myself feel better about it?’).
The key construct within the CSM is the idea of illness representations or ‘lay’ beliefs about illness. These representations integrate with existing schemata (the normative guidelines that people hold), enabling them to make sense of their symptoms and guide any coping actions. Leventhal and colleagues [2] describe five components of these illness representations:
-
Identity: the label or name given to the condition and the symptoms that ‘appear’ to go with it. It can be argued that people like to have a label for their symptoms (for legitimization) although, conversely, once given, people are likely to interpret diverse symptoms as evidence of the label [6].
-
Cause: the individualistic ideas about the perceived cause of the condition, which may not be completely biomedically accurate. These representations will be based on information gathered from personal experience as well as the opinions and discourses of significant others, health professionals and media sources, reflecting issues such as stress, environmental pollution and other pathogens.
-
Time-line: the predictive belief about how long the condition might last, i.e. is it acute or is it going to be chronic? These beliefs will be re-evaluated as time progresses, and it has been suggested that ‘Inside every chronic patient is an acute patient wondering what happened’ [9].
-
Consequences: the individual beliefs about the consequences of the condition and how this will impact on them physically and socially. These representations may only develop into more realistic beliefs over time.
-
Curability/controllability: the beliefs about whether the condition can be cured or kept under control and the degree to which the individual plays a part in achieving this.
As people with a chronic illness like hand osteoarthritis obtain new information about their condition and evaluate their attempts to moderate, cure or cope with its effects, new representations are formed and develop based upon these experiences. Illness representations are in effect cumulative, with information being adopted, discarded or adapted as necessary. These representations are, therefore, expected to be linked to the selection of coping procedures, action plans and outcomes. For example, focusing on dietary content and control is a way people with osteoarthritis attempt to cure/control their symptoms [10].
It is important for clinicians to appreciate the multiple levels at which these representations operate and how they guide patients’ preferences for treatment and the behaviors in which they engage over time. Furthermore, representations will operate at an abstract and experiential level, and are likely to be weighted in importance in a different way to health professionals. For instance, patients’ expectations about the timeline for the effectiveness of medications are likely to develop from the abstract, e.g. ‘I’ll feel better once I’ve started the treatment,’ to the experiential, e.g. ‘I haven’t noticed any difference yet since I started treatment,’ whilst the clinician focuses on the actual, e.g. ‘It will take 3 months for this treatment to start to take effect’ [3].
Although Leventhal and colleagues’ original research [6, 3] used semi-structured interviews to identify the dimensions of illness representations, the utility of the CSM has been investigated extensively in recent years following the development of a questionnaire, the Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ) [11], which addresses the five dimensions detailed above. This questionnaire has been revised and expanded (and renamed the IPQ-R) [12], as applied in Hill et al.’s study [1].
Researchers have been keen to determine how illness representations might influence both coping and outcomes in a diverse range of chronic diseases, including chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) [13], Addison’s disease [14], psoriasis [15], multiple sclerosis [16] and rheumatoid arthritis (RA) [17, 18]. The premise is that illness representations are related to coping efforts, and via these efforts to outcomes, i.e. coping acts as a mediator. However, research currently seems to indicate that illness representations may be associated with outcomes ‘relatively’ independent of the coping strategies used [19]. However, this conclusion must be cautious: the direction of relationships might not be clear-cut as coping responses could influence illness representations which may then feedback to influence choice of coping strategy [13]. In our research in RA [18], avoidant coping did not predict fatigue after 1yr, and this coping style did not appear to be spurred by perceiving that RA had serious consequences; these perceptions of consequences did have a moderate direct effect on change in fatigue though. We believe these results could be an artefact of the coping subscales used, in that they simply are not sensitive enough and may not be culturally relevant having been created and validated in the United States. Studies addressing the content and face validity of coping measures among British patients may therefore be enlightening.
It is likely that other factors influence the proposed pathway from representation to outcome via coping. For example, the CSM does not make specific predictions about the role of and interaction with significant others. Few of us are hermits, and the input from family, friends and caregivers may directly or indirectly affect adjustment. This consideration is also extremely important when research samples are drawn from established self-management groups, charitable organizations and established disease populations such as attendees at hospital out-patient clinics, which may already have gone some way to influencing disease knowledge and coping strategies. This is one of the reasons Hill et al.’s study [1] is a positive development in illness representation research, in that it is sited within the community, drawing upon a large sample of people, who may (or may not) define themselves as having osteoarthritis and may not have a clinical diagnosis.
In drawing a large sample of middle- to older-aged adults from the community, Hill et al. [1] return to the heart of the conceptualization of the CSM, which looked at the process that people went through in searching for a ‘label’ or identity for their condition (albeit not necessarily biomedically correct). Therefore, there may be those who ‘think’ they have osteoarthritis but have never had a diagnosis of such a condition; the importance is in their ‘perception’ that they have it and the ways they have adapted their lives accordingly.
The ultimate goal, of course, for health professionals is to determine whether in understanding illness representations we might be able to predict certain outcomes that are then potentially alterable in a favorable way for the individual, the service provided and the social situation of the patient. For instance, in order to develop interventions to enhance attendance at cardiac rehabilitation, Whitmarsh et al. [20] used the IPQ to help them distinguish between people who attend frequently and those who attend rarely or not at all. It follows that illness representations form cognitions that are then potentially alterable. This is the premise upon which self-management groups such as the Arthritis Self-Management Program and the Chronic Disease Self-Management Course are built [21–22]. Hence, disease outcomes might be positively influenced by cognitive-behavioral interventions.
Although there has been much use of the IPQ to examine the relationship between representations, coping efforts and outcomes, it may be more useful to look at how we might apply our knowledge about individual representations to guide the development of interventional programs, as Hill et al. [1] and other researchers have noted.
A recent article by Stafford entitled ‘Isn’t it all just obvious?’ [23] made us reflect on the wider messages that Hill et al. convey in their paper [1]. As Stafford put it: ‘It’s all just common sense, but dressed up with big words to confuse people’ [23]. Indeed the core of the CSM is that people with an illness use common sense in constructing their representations based on their knowledge and experiences. People actively test out these common-sense hypotheses by their use of emotion- and problem-focused coping efforts, just as in our role as scientists we test our hypotheses in order to determine whether what we ‘think’ we know is justified. And as with illness representations, scientific evidence is open to debate and disagreement. Patients may not be consciously aware of how good they are at testing their illness hypotheses, and with careful explanation and guidance they can be facilitated in constructing useful representations that assist in positive outcomes. Cognitive behavioral therapies (CBT) are growing in application within the UK healthcare system. In the field of rheumatology, positive results have been recorded for tailored CBT interventions among individuals with RA and systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE). In particular, reduced fatigue, depression and helplessness and enhanced use of active coping strategies have been facilitated in RA [24]. The illness representations model has been used explicitly to tailor similar CBT interventions in pilot studies led by psychologists. Among people with lupus, a self-selected controlled trial of an intervention program by Goodman et al. [25] containing a module to explain and attempt to improve illness representations managed to enhance participants’ perceptions that treatment can control their lupus and reduced their perceptions of the emotional impact of their lupus and their overall stress. Among people who had recently experienced an acute myocardial infarction by Petrie et al. [26], perceptions of chronic time-line, serious consequences and lack of control were shown to be modifiable in a randomized controlled trial of an intervention, and this led to improved symptoms of angina and ability to return to employment. Expansion of this approach is ripe for individualizing existing patient ‘self-management’ education and given the extent of hand problems and the role of illness perceptions discovered in Hill et al.’s study [1], this would be an ideal condition with which to take this forward.
Candela Citations
- The Common-Sense Model of self-regulation of health and illness: how can we use it to understand and respond to our patientsu2019 needs?. Authored by: E. D. Hale, G. J. Treharne, G. D. Kitas, The Common-Sense Model of self-regulation of health and illness: how can we use it to understand and respond to our patientsu2019 needs?, Rheumatology, Volume 46, Issue 6, June 2007, Pages 904u2013906, . Provided by: The Author 2007. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the British Society for Rheumatology. All rights reserved. . Located at: https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/kem060. License: All Rights Reserved